Cognitive Psychology For Dummies®
Published by: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, www.wiley.com
This edition first published 2016
© 2016 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Chichester, West Sussex
Registered Office
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, United Kingdom
For details of our global editorial offices, for customer services and for information about how to apply for permission to reuse the copyright material in this book, please see our website at www.wiley.com
.
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, without the permission of this publisher.
Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. All brand names and product names used in this book are trade names, service marks, trademarks or registered trademarks of their respective owners. The publisher is not associated with any product or vendor mentioned in this book.
LIMIT OF LIABILITY/DISCLAIMER OF WARRANTY: THE CONTENTS OF THIS WORK ARE INTENDED TO FURTHER GENERAL SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH, UNDERSTANDING, AND DISCUSSION ONLY AND ARE NOT INTENDED AND SHOULD NOT BE RELIED UPON AS RECOMMENDING OR PROMOTING A SPECIFIC METHOD, DIAGNOSIS, OR TREATMENT BY PHYSICIANS FOR ANY PARTICULAR PATIENT. THE PUBLISHER AND THE AUTHOR MAKE NO REPRESENTATIONS OR WARRANTIES WITH RESPECT TO THE ACCURACY OR COMPLETENESS OF THE CONTENTS OF THIS WORK AND SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ALL WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION ANY IMPLIED WARRANTIES OF FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. IN VIEW OF ONGOING RESEARCH, EQUIPMENT MODIFICATIONS, CHANGES IN GOVERNMENTAL REGULATIONS, AND THE CONSTANT FLOW OF INFORMATION, THE READER IS URGED TO REVIEW AND EVALUATE THE INFORMATION PROVIDED IN THE PACKAGE INSERT OR INSTRUCTIONS FOR EACH MEDICINE, EQUIPMENT, OR DEVICE FOR, AMONG OTHER THINGS, ANY CHANGES IN THE INSTRUCTIONS OR INDICATION OF USAGE AND FOR ADDED WARNINGS AND PRECAUTIONS. READERS SHOULD CONSULT WITH A SPECIALIST WHERE APPROPRIATE. NEITHER THE PUBLISHER NOR THE AUTHOR SHALL BE LIABLE FOR ANY DAMAGES ARISING HEREFROM.
For general information on our other products and services, please contact our Customer Care Department within the U.S. at 877-762-2974, outside the U.S. at 317-572-3993, or fax 317-572-4002. For technical support, please visit www.wiley.com/techsupport
.
Wiley publishes in a variety of print and electronic formats and by print-on-demand. Some material included with standard print versions of this book may not be included in e-books or in print-on-demand. If this book refers to media such as a CD or DVD that is not included in the version you purchased, you may download this material at http://booksupport.wiley.com
. For more information about Wiley products, visit www.wiley.com
.
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.
Library of Congress Control Number: 2016930757
ISBN 978-1-119-95321-0 (pbk); ISBN 978-1-119-95390-6 (ebk); ISBN 978-1-119-95391-3 (ebk)
The fact that you’re reading this book implies that you have an interest in cognitive psychology or you’re studying it for a course. In either case, you probably think that you know what cognitive psychology is: the study of all mental abilities and processes about knowing. Clearly, the subject covers a huge range whose contents would barely fit into 50 books the size of this one – with more being written every day!
We think that everybody should be interested in cognitive psychology, because it’s fascinating. We know that all aficionados say that (from bell ringers to beer-mat collectors), but cognitive psychology really is! By scientifically studying how people see, remember, know, speak and think, you can truly understand what being human means and what makes all humans special.
Cognitive Psychology For Dummies is designed as an introduction to the subject. We cover the historical perspective on cognitive psychology, but also draw on interesting, more recent work.
We adopt an informal writing style, but one that remains technically appropriate and scientifically accurate. We write in plain English (which is tricky, because cognitive psychologists love jargon!). Where we do use technical language you can assume that it’s the only way to express something, but overall we make the tone as friendly as possible. We even include some jokes (if you don’t spot any, it’s because we’re not very funny!). At no point are we making fun of anyone (except ourselves).
We endeavour to relate everything in this book to everyday reality, using real-world examples to anchor the more technical information. Nevertheless, cognitive psychologists like to create highly controlled, laboratory-based experiments that, on the surface, bear little resemblance to the real world. Don’t fear though; everything cognitive psychologists study has some benefit to humanity.
Most chapters also cover instances of ‘when things go wrong’. These discussions show how a particular cognitive ability can go haywire in healthy people (such as visual illusions) or those with brain damage.
This book is for people who need and want to know about cognitive psychology. For the former, we present all the information covered in school and the first year of a university course (anywhere in the world) in a highly accessible way. We map the content onto the most common courses of cognitive psychology. If you simply want to know about cognitive psychology, we present some of the most interesting and fun psychology here too. We pack the book with examples and exercises you can try out and demonstrate on your friends and family to amaze them!
We use conventions to help you find your way around this book easily:
Unlike most textbooks in psychology, we don’t include references or in-text citations. We mention the name of a researcher when we feel that the person’s work is important and worth remembering.
We sometimes describe a few of the most important and influential studies, but not always. Be assured, however, that all the results and effects we describe in this book are based on empirical research – we simply don’t want to get bogged down in such detail too often.
We also provide a number of sidebars, containing additional information with more detailed theories, methodologies or clinical examples. You can skip over these without missing anything essential, but we think they’re interesting and add a lot to the text.
Hundreds of books on cognitive psychology exist. Many are technical, long, dry, specialised or cover a very narrow area of cognition. We wrote Cognitive Psychology For Dummies assuming the following:
Throughout this book, we use icons in the margins to help you find certain types of information. Here’s a list of what they mean.
The area of cognitive psychology is so vast that its contents would fill far more than this book. Given that it’s really interesting and exciting, we want to give you as much chance to learn about it as possible, and so we put some extras on the Internet. In addition to the printed chapters, you can find loads more (free!) Cognitive Psychology For Dummies information at www.dummies.com/extras/cognitivepsychology
.
In an online cheat sheet found at www.dummies.com/cheatsheet/cognitivepsychology
, we include a quick guide to some central cognitive psychology ideas on memory, language and problem solving, among other topics.
We organise this book in a logical representation of how the human brain works (information comes in, is remembered, spoken and thought about), but each chapter is self-contained so that you can dip in and out at your leisure. Except for the first and last parts, each part deals with a different element of cognitive psychology, so you can pick out the sections that you’re most interested in or are struggling with the most.
Use the table of contents and index to find what’s most relevant to you. If you’re new to the subject, you may want to start with Chapter 1 and read the book in sequence, but you don’t have to read it cover to cover.
We hope that you find the book educational, informative and entertaining. We think that you’ll like it and learn a lot about yourself as you go. If you do, tell your friends about it!
Part I
In this part …
Understand what cognitive psychology is and why it’s so darn important.
Realise how cognitive psychology influences every aspect of the human experience that involves thinking.
Find useful tips on how cognitive psychology can improve your cognitive skills in school, college, university and almost all walks of life.
Chapter 1
In This Chapter
Defining cognitive psychology
Detailing the discipline’s research methods
Looking at some limitations
How do you know that what you see is real? Would you notice if someone changed her identity in front of you? How can you be sure that when you remember what you saw, you’re remembering it accurately? Plus, how can you be sure that when you tell someone something that the person understands it in the same way as you do? What’s more fascinating than looking for answers to such questions, which lie at the heart of what it means to be … well … you!
Cognitive psychology is the study of all mental abilities and processes about knowing. Despite the huge area of concern that this description implies, the breadth of the subject’s focus still sometimes surprises people. Here, we introduce you to cognitive psychology, suggesting that it’s fundamentally a science. We show how cognitive psychologists view the subject from an information-processing account and how we use this view to structure this book.
We also describe the plethora of research methods that psychologists employ to study cognitive psychology. The rest of this book uses the philosophies and methods that we describe here, and so this chapter works as an introduction to the book as well.
To help define cognitive psychology and demonstrate its ‘scientificness’, we need to define what we mean by a science and then look at the history of cognitive psychology within this context.
An example may be: how do people store information in their memory? Sometimes this is called a model (you encounter many models in this book).
Create a situation to see whether the hypothesis is true: that is, manipulate something and see what it affects.
Philosopher Karl Popper suggested that science progresses faster when people devise tests to prove hypotheses wrong: called falsification. After you prove all but one hypothesis wrong about something, you have the answer (the Sherlock Holmes approach – if you exclude the impossible, whatever remains must be true!). This is also called deductive reasoning (see Chapter 18 for the psychology of deduction).
The scientific method has some clear and obvious limitations (or strengths, depending on the way you look at it):
Before cognitive psychology, people used a variety of approaches (or paradigms) to study psychology, including behaviourism, psychophysics and psychodynamics. The year 1956, however, saw the start of a cognitive renaissance, which challenged, in particular, behaviourism. For more background on how cognitive psychology emerged from other scientific disciplines, chiefly behaviourism, check out the nearby sidebar ‘1956: The year cognitive psychology was born’.
We don’t intend to minimise the importance of behaviourism: it ensured that the scientific method was applied to psychology and that experiments were conducted in a controlled way. Cognitive psychology took this strength and carried it into more ingenious scientific studies of cognition.
Fittingly, we’re writing this book to bring cognitive psychology to a wider audience around the 50th anniversary of the first published cognitive psychology textbook (in 1967).
In Part I, we review the applications of cognitive psychology and why studying it is important. Cognitive psychology has produced some incredibly exciting and interesting findings that have changed how people view psychology and themselves (as you can discover in Chapter 2). But also, people have learnt a great deal about how best to teach, learn and improve themselves from cognitive psychology, something we address in Chapter 3. The applications of cognitive psychology are so wide that studies are used in such disparate fields as computing, social work, education, media technology, human resources and much more besides.
© John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Figure 1-1: Basic processes in cognition according to information processing.
All cognition fits within this framework. Cognitive psychologists research each box (stage) and each arrow (process) in Figure 1-1 in many different domains. In other words, this framework provides a good structure for how to think about and learn about cognitive psychology (and oddly matches the framework of this book).
All forms of cognitive psychology are based on the interaction between bottom-up and top-down processing. No processing is strictly driven by the stimulus or by knowledge.
Cognitive psychologists like the information-processing framework, because people’s interactions with the world are guided by internal mental representations (such as language) that can be revealed by measuring the processing time. Neuroscientists have also found parts of the brain responsible for different cognitive behaviours.
In Part II of this book, we look at the first stage of cognition: input of information. In the computer analogy, this would be a camera recording information or the keyboard receiving key presses.
Attention follows information input (see Chapter 7). Attention is the first distinct process of the information-processing account, and it’s what links perception with higher-level cognition. Without it, people would simply react to the world in an involuntary manner.
After you attend to information, it enters your brain’s storage system (see the chapters in Part III). The brain has a number of mechanisms for storing and using information, collectively called memory. We cover short-term memory in Chapter 8 and long-term memory in Chapter 9. You also have stored knowledge and skills (Chapter 10). Although all this knowledge is highly useful, we can’t forget(!) to consider forgetting (Chapter 11), as well as how memory works in everyday life and some of the applications of memory research (Chapter 12).
Sensation and perception are quite low-level cognitive functions: they’re fairly simple processes that many animals can do. Memory is a slightly higher-level cognitive function, but the highest-level functions are the ones that animals can’t do, according to some psychologists – language and thought (see Parts IV and V):
Language: The first output stage of information processing. Some psychologists describe it as a human form of communication and it’s typically the vocal form of exchanging ideas with other people. We describe language and its relation to other forms of communication in Chapter 13. We cover its structure and the steps needed to produce it in Chapters 14 and 15. We discuss how language relates to other parts of cognition and perception in Chapter 16.
People have devised a number of methods for researching cognitive psychology. Plus, technological advances allow psychologists to explore how the brain functions. In this section, we describe how experiments, computational models, work with patients and brain scanning helped psychologists to understand how the cognitive system works.
The tightly controlled laboratory experiment is one of the most commonly used techniques for researching cognitive psychology. Psychologists take normal people (like those exist!) – usually university students (narrowing the definition of normal to those generally well-educated and intelligent) – place these participants in small cubicles and show them things on a computer. Each person is tested in exactly the same way and the experimenters have complete control over what the person sees (as long as the computers follow the given instructions!).
The experimenters take the participants’ responses, usually in terms of measures of response speed and their accuracy, and use statistics to work out whether the hypothesis and cognitive psychological model is correct or not. These statistics allow researchers to see whether the sample tested reflects the whole population of people that could’ve been tested. Then the psychologists tell the world!
© John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Figure 1-2: A basic connectionist model. Usually the top layer is the output, the middle layer is hidden processing and the bottom layer is the input.
Production models are based around formal logic (Chapter 18). They rely on a series of ‘if … then’ statements. The idea is that stored knowledge exists in terms of ‘if this happens, then this will’. Another technique – artificial intelligence – involves constructing a computer to produce intelligent outcomes, though it doesn’t have to reflect human processing.
Often, neuropsychologists use case studies. They look at individuals with a certain type of brain damage to understand what different parts of the brain do to a wide range of tasks. Certain people have been extensively researched and so have contributed to the knowledge of the brain more than many researchers! Chapter 21 has ten case studies for you to read.
The German neurologist, Korbinian Brodmann, was the first to map the brain directly. He named 52 different brain areas and his descriptions are still used today. The assumption is that each area does a slightly different thing (based on the modularity assumption of the cognitive neuropsychologists we describe in the preceding section).
These techniques can be useful in establishing which part of the brain is responsible for processing certain things, although none of them are completely accurate. To use neuroimaging techniques appropriately, you need to run a good, well-controlled cognitive test that really measures only one ability (to pinpoint which part of the brain is responsible for that ability – see the next section).
Cognitive psychologists’ clever experiments (refer to the preceding section) have produced exciting findings that can help society greatly. We even use evidence from cognitive psychological research in Chapter 3 to help you in your studies!
Task impurity: Many tasks that cognitive psychologists devise may not measure only the one intended aspect. For example, a researcher may be interested in response inhibition and use the Go/No-Go test (see Chapter 8), but this task also involves response conflict (a related, but subtly different cognitive process). The researcher’s results may therefore reflect two different types of cognition, which is called task impurity.
Furthermore, results from one task are sometimes not repeated in a similar task. This paradigm specificity reflects the problem that some cognitive psychological effects are limited to the very precise experimental procedures used to find them.
Lack of ecological validity: In the attempt to be highly scientific, psychologists take people out of the real world and create artificial environments where they control every aspect of their behaviour. This is unrealistic, and so results may not occur in the real world.
Cognitive psychologists are interested in the internal mental processes that occur during cognition, but these processes aren’t directly observable. As a result, the evidence they collect is only indirect. Indeed, many cognitive psychologists’ theories are limited in scope and only focus on a small aspect of the human experience. Therefore, many areas of cognitive psychology don’t relate to other areas of cognitive psychology.
Chapter 2
In This Chapter
Seeing the central areas of cognitive psychology
Understanding that cognition can go wrong
Appreciating that the point lies in the journey
When people think of psychology they tend to focus on the more unusual cases and findings, such as explaining unusual behaviour. But psychology is concerned with every aspect of people’s mental life, not just the extremes. The contents of most cognitive psychology books feature apparently mundane topics – such as seeing, remembering, using language and thinking – things that people do all the time without a second thought.
This focus on everyday behaviour is useful, because when you’re studying any subject you improve best when it’s relevant to you. Therefore, a good way to get to grips with cognitive psychology’s topics is to think about how they relate to your everyday life and how you can use the knowledge to improve the way you do things. Fortunately, cognitive psychologists have come up with a whole range of fascinating findings that alter how people view themselves. In this chapter, you read about just some of the many ways in which cognitive psychology plays an important role in the real world – as well as its limitations.